This is not the post I thought I’d be writing today. Two weeks ago, when I started to try to understand willful ignorance, I was mad. I suspect I shared that feeling with many of you. I was
tired of the deliberate denial of fact that had consequences for all of us. I was frustrated with anti-masking, anti-vaxxing, anti-climate change and, most of all, anti-science. I was ready to go to
war with those I saw in the other camp.
And that, I found out, is exactly the problem. Let me explain.
First, to recap. As I talked about two weeks ago, willful ignorance is a decision based on beliefs, so it’s
very difficult -- if not impossible -- to argue, cajole or inform people out of it. And, as I wrote last week, willful ignorance has some very real and damaging consequences.
advertisement
advertisement
This post was supposed to talk about what we do about that problem. I intended to find ways to isolate the impact of willful ignorance and minimize its downside. In doing so, I was going to suggest
putting up even more walls to separate “us” from “them.”
But the more I researched this and thought about it, the more I realized that that was exactly the wrong
approach. Because this recent plague of willful ignorance is many things -- but most of all, it’s one more example of how we love to separate “us” from “them.” And both
sides, including mine, are equally guilty of doing this.
In a normal life, we are constantly rubbing elbows with those of all leanings. And, if we want to function in that life, we have to
find a way to get along with everyone, even if we don’t like them or agree with them. For most of us, that natural and temporary social bonding is something we haven’t had to do much
lately.
It’s this lowering of our ideological defense systems that starts to bridge the gaps between us and them. And it also starts pumping oxygen into our ideological vacuums, prying
the lids off our air-tight belief systems. A little trust between those with different beliefs can go a long way.
After World War II, psychologists and sociologists started to pick apart a
fundamental question: How, in the name of humanity, did the atrocities of the war occur? They began vigorously to explore this fundamental human need to sort ourselves into the categories of
“us” and “them.”
In the 1970s, psychologist Henri Tajfel found that we barely need a nudge to start
creating in-groups and out-groups. We’ll do it for anything, even something as trivial as which abstract artist, Klee or Kandisky, we prefer.
Once sorted on the flimsiest of premises,
these groups in the experiment started showing a strong preference to favor their own group and punish the other. There was no preexisting animosity between the groups, but when playing various games,
they showed that they would even forego rewards for themselves if it meant depriving the other group of their share.
If we do this for completely arbitrary reasons such as those used by
Tajfel, imagine how nasty we can get when the stakes are much higher, such as our own health or the future of the planet.
So if we naturally sort ourselves into in groups and out groups, and
our likelihood to consider perspectives other than our own increases the more we’re exposed to those perspectives in a non-hostile environment, how do we start taking down those walls?
Here’s where it gets interesting.
What we need to break down the walls between “us” and “them” is to find another “them” that we can then unite
against.
One of the theories about why the U.S. is so polarized now is that with the end of the Cold War, the U.S. lost a common enemy that united “us” in opposition to
“them.” Without the USSR, our naturally tendency to categorize ourselves into in groups and out groups had no option but to turn inwards.
You might think this is hogwash, but
before you throw me into the “them” camp, let me tell you about what happened in Robbers Cave State Park in Oklahoma.
One of the experiments into this ingroup/outgroup phenomenon was conducted by psychologist Muzafer Sherif in the summer of 1954. He and
his associates took 22 boys of similar backgrounds to a summer camp at Robbers Cave and randomly divided them into two groups. First, they built team loyalty and then they gradually introduced a
competitive environment between the two groups. Predictably, animosity and prejudice soon developed between them.
Sherif and his assistants then introduced a four-day cooling off period and
then tried to reduce conflict by mixing the two groups. It didn’t work. In fact, it just made things worse.
Things didn’t improve until the two groups were brought together to
overcome a common obstacle, when the experimenters purposely sabotaged the camp’s water supply. Suddenly, the two groups came together to overcome a bigger challenge.
This, by the way,
is exactly the same theory behind the process that NASA and Amazon’s Blue
Origin use to build trust in their flight crews.
As I said, when I started this journey, I was squarely in the “us” vs “them” camp. To be honest, I’m still
fighting my instinct to stay there. But I don’t think that’s the best way forward.
I’m hoping that as our world inches towards a better state of normal, everyday life will
start to force the camps together and our evolved instincts for cooperation will begin again.
I also believe that the past 19 months (and counting) will be a period that sociologists and
psychologists will study for years to come, as it’s been an ongoing experiment in human behavior at a scope that may never happen again.
We can certainly hope so.